Inconsistency: The Real Hobgoblin
by JON
HAMILTON, ALIX SPIEGEL AND SHANKAR VEDANTAM
Jae
C. Hong/AP
Republican
presidential candidates Rick Santorum and Mitt Romney clashed often during
Wednesday's GOP debate.
This
campaign season, inconsistency seems to be, well, almost everywhere. Each
flip-flopping politician revels in pointing out the flip-flopping ways of his
opponents. Why are politicians and those of us who vote for them so obsessed
with inconsistency? We take that question on from three angles: how our brains
are wired; the psychology of judging what's consistent; and how consistency
plays out in leadership styles.
Jon Hamilton: Why Our Brains Hate A Flip-Flopper
The
problem with flip-floppers is that they are, by definition, inconsistent. They're
unpredictable.
And
our brains don't like that, says David Linden, a professor
of neuroscience at Johns Hopkins.
Take The Flip-Flop Quiz
Match The
Presidential Candidate to the Change Of Heart to understand why,
Linden says, it helps to consider how the brain looks for consistency and
predictability in even a mundane event like reaching for a cup of coffee. Long
before your hand reaches the cup, your brain starts making predictions about
everything from how much force will be required to lift the cup to how the
coffee will taste. Once the brain makes its predictions, it starts to "use
sensory information as it comes in to compare the prediction with what actually
took place," Linden says. You grasp. You smell. You taste. If the cup's
weight and the coffee's flavor match the predictions, your brain declares
victory. If not, it tries to figure out what went wrong. Of course, coffee
forecasting is pretty low stakes. Linden says.
Your Stories
But
we have lots of brain circuits that are making predictions about all kinds of
things, every second of every day. And the brain pays special attention to
other people, Linden says. "We're extremely attuned to the veracity, and
the predictability, and the group spirit and the motivations of those around
us," he says
That's
probably from thousands of years living in groups. To stay alive, we had to
know if the person who helped us yesterday might hurt us tomorrow. Prediction
is so important that our brains actually give us a chemical reward when we do
it well, Linden says.
"We
are intrinsically wired to take pleasure from our predictions that come
true," he says. Get it right and you get a burst of pleasure-inducing
dopamine or a related brain chemical. Get it wrong and dopamine levels dip,
Linden says. All that training makes us extremely sensitive to the consistency
and predictability of people we depend on, Linden says.
"If
we have a sense that there is a mismatch between our prediction and their
actions, that is something that sets off neural alarm bells," he says. And
if we think they have been inconsistent about something fundamental, he says,
we will feel betrayed. "When we feel deeply betrayed, either by a leader,
or by someone in our social circle, or by our beloved, that pain really is
similar to physical pain," Linden says.
In
other words, we're hard-wired to suffer from the inconsistency of
flip-floppers. No wonder we don't like them.
The Flip-Flop Quiz: Match The Presidential Candidate
To The Change Of Heart
The
"flip-flopper" label can be deadly on the campaign trail (see: John
Kerry, 2004). As voters, we value consistency in our politicians. We want to
make sure they do what they say and say what they mean. Alas, politicians don't
always oblige. Play our quiz below: Match the quote to the politician, and
we'll tell you how he later flipped on the issue.
Credit: JoElla Straley / NPR
Alix Spiegel: How Your Tribe Affects Your View
My
colleague Jon Hamilton makes the argument that we don't like flip-flopping. But
this flip flopping thing? It's really in the eye of the beholder.
Consider
the work of Jamie Barden,
a psychology professor a Howard University in Washington.
Barden
found a clever way to look at how people make judgments about inconsistent
behavior in politics.
In
one study, Barden gathered a group of students, both Democrats and Republicans,
and told them that their job was to evaluate the behavior of a political
fundraiser named Mike.
The
first piece of information the students got about Mike was that after a long
night of drinking at a fundraiser he'd organized, Mike drove home and wrapped his
car around a telephone pole. Then they found out that about a month after the
crash, Mike had gone on the radio and delivered a screed about the dangers of
drunken driving. Mike had driven while drunk, then Mike had preached against
drunken driving.
The
students were then provided with a blank space and the opportunity to weigh in
on Mike's behavior.
Now
obviously there are two possible interpretations of Mike's actions. The first
interpretation is that Mike is a hypocrite. Privately he's driving into poles.
Publicly he's making proclamations. He's a person whose public and private
behavior is inconsistent. The other interpretation is that Mike is a changed
man. Mike had a hard experience. Mike learned. Mike grew. So when do we see
hypocrisy and when do we see growth?
What
Barden found is that this decision is based much less on the facts of what
happened, than on tribe.
Half
the time the hypothetical Mike was described to the students in the study as a Republicans,
and half the time he was described as a Democrat.
When
participants were making judgments of a Mike who was in their own party, only
16 percent found him to be a hypocrite. When participants were making judgments
about a Mike from the opposing party, 40 percent found him to be a hypocrite.
In
other words our judgments about what is inconsistent and what isn't are clouded
by our social allegiances. In fact, the research makes it clear it is
hopelessly clouded. Further, there's a whole other school of research that
shows that though we can often see this bias in our opponents, we are blind to
the behavior in ourselves. We believe that we are earnestly making judgements
based on facts, on reality.
So
during this campaign season watch the way you watch. Are you seeing hypocrisy
where there's growth? Or maybe growth where there's hypocrisy?
Shankar Vedantam: The Political Consequences Of
Consistency
A
desire for consistency is wired into the brain, which is why we care so much
about politicians being consistent.
We're
also inconsistent about how we perceive consistency. We're more likely to spot
it in our opponents than among our friends or ourselves. But what are the
consequences of consistency? Voters say they want consistent leaders: What
happens when they get what they want?
Psychologist Philip
Tetlock, at the Wharton School of Business at the University of
Pennsylvania, has worked on an enormous research project for decades that can
help answer that question. He's based some of his conclusions on an ancient
aphorism from the Greek warriot-poet Archilochus:
"The fox knows many things, but the hedgehog knows one big thing."
A foolish consistency is the hobgoblin of little
minds, adored by little statesmen and philosophers and divines.
- Ralph Waldo Emerson
Tetlock
applies this analogy to politics. The hedgehog has one goal: It doesn't want to
get eaten. Foxes, on the other hand, are crafty. They have lots of strategies
to catch a hedgehog. Tetlock thinks consistent leaders simplify a complex world
into a few big ideas. That's why he thinks they're like hedgehogs.
"There
are many different types of hedgehogs," Tetlock says. "You could be
on the left or the right. You could be a free-market hedgehog, or you could be
a Keynesian hedgehog or even a socialist or Marxist hedgehog."
By
contrast, leaders who are foxes don't have a single agenda. They have lots of
contradictory goals. They support government spending in one case; oppose it in
another. They compromise. "Foxes, on average, are more likely to be
neither extreme boomsters nor extreme doomsters," says Tetlock. "They
are less likely to be on the extreme left or the extreme right."
Consistent
hedgehogs and inconsistent foxes both claim great results, so Tetlock has put
their claims to the test. He asked a large number of hedgehogs and foxes to
make specific predictions about events. Over 20 years, he's collected more than
28,000 predictions about issues in 60 countries. The results are in: Foxes make
the right calls more often than hedgehogs. If you want to know where the
economy's headed, ask a fox.
But
hedgehogs have a curious quality. The upside is when they're right, they're
spectacularly right. Think of Winston Churchill, who saw the threat of Hitler
before everyone else. The downside is hedgehogs are also more likely to be
spectacularly wrong.
"Churchill
had a low threshold for seeing threats to the British Empire," Tetlock
says. "He saw Gandhi as a terrible threat and he actually made comparisons
between Hitler and Gandhi; comparisons that now we would regard as historically
embarrassing."
Tetlock's
work intersects with other research that shows leaders with inconsistent
worldviews tend to do better in office. But leaders who have a clear,
consistent message do better during campaigns.
The
best presidents, Tetlock says, may be foxes — who disguise themselves.
"They
campaign like hedgehogs, and they govern like foxes," Tetlock says.
So
when you next hear a politician with a clear, consistent message, ask yourself,
is this a hedgehog, or a fox in hedgehog's clothing?
No comments:
Post a Comment